Our eyes are drawn to meaning, not shiny objects

(Credit: Getty Images)

Our visual attention homes in on the parts of a scene that have meaning, not those that “stick out” most, new research suggests.

Our eyes perceive a wide field of view in front of us, but we only focus our attention on a small part of this field. How do we decide where to direct our attention, without thinking about it?


The dominant theory in attention studies has been “visual salience,” says John Henderson, a psychology professor from the University of California, Davis who led the new research. Salience means things that “stick out” from the background, like colorful berries on a background of leaves or a brightly lit object in a room.

The new findings, however, overturn this widely-held model of visual attention. “A lot of people will have to rethink things,” Henderson says. “The saliency hypothesis really is the dominant view.”

Saliency is relatively easy to measure. You can map the amount of saliency in different areas of a picture by measuring relative contrast or brightness, for example.

Henderson called this the “magpie theory”: Our attention is drawn to bright and shiny objects.

“It becomes obvious, though, that it can’t be right,” he says, otherwise we would constantly be distracted.

Henderson and postdoctoral researcher Taylor Hayes set out to test whether attention is guided instead by how “meaningful” we find an area within our view. They first had to construct “meaning maps” of test scenes, where different parts of the scene had different levels of meaning to an observer.

To make their meaning maps, Henderson and Hayes took images of scenes, broke them up into overlapping circular tiles, and submitted the individual tiles to the online crowdsourcing service Mechanical Turk, asking users to rate the tiles for meaning.

attention and meaning maps
Researchers mapped hundreds of images (far left) by eye tracking (center left), “meaning” (center right) and salience or outstanding features (far left). Statistical analysis shows that eyes are drawn to “meaningful” areas, not necessarily those that are most outstanding. (Credit: John Henderson and Taylor Hayes/UC Davis)

Based on the voting results, the researchers assigned levels of meaning to different areas of images and created meaning maps comparable to saliency maps of the same scenes.

Next, they tracked the eye movements of volunteers as they looked at the scenes. Those eyetracks gave them maps of what parts of images attracted the most attention. These “attention maps” were closer to the meaning maps than the saliency maps, Henderson says.

Henderson and Hayes don’t yet have firm data on what makes part of a scene meaningful, although they have some ideas. For example, a cluttered table or shelf attracted more attention than a highly salient splash of sunlight on a wall. With further work, they hope to develop a “taxonomy of meaning,” Henderson says.

Although the research is aimed at a fundamental understanding of how visual attention works, there could be some near-term applications, Henderson says, for example, in developing automated visual systems that allow computers to scan security footage or to automatically identify or caption images online.

Babies pay attention when parents do, too

The researchers report their work in the journal Nature Human Behavior.

The National Science Foundation supported the work.

Source: UC Davis