A new model of emotion in the brain shows that a current treatment for the most common mental health problems could be ineffective or even detrimental to left-handers.
Since the 1970s, hundreds of studies have suggested that each hemisphere of the brain is home to a specific type of emotion. The neural system for emotions linked to approaching and engaging with the world—like happiness, pride, and anger—lives in the left side of the brain, while emotions associated with avoidance—like disgust and fear—are housed in the right.
But those studies were done almost exclusively on right-handed people—a simple fact that gives a skewed understanding of how emotion works in the brain, says Daniel Casasanto, associate professor of human development and of psychology at Cornell University.
That long-standing model is, in fact, reversed in left-handed people, whose emotions like alertness and determination are housed in the right side of their brains, Casasanto suggests in a new study.
Further, the location of a person’s neural systems for emotion depends on whether they are left-handed, right-handed, or somewhere in between, the research shows.
“The old model suggests that each hemisphere is specialized for one type of emotion, but that’s not true,” Casasanto says. “Approach emotions are smeared over both hemispheres according to the direction and degree of your handedness. The big theoretical shift is, we’re saying emotion in the brain isn’t its own system. Emotion in the cerebral cortex is built upon neural systems for motor action.”
Sword and shield
The idea for the researchers’ theory, called the “sword and shield” hypothesis, stems from Casasanto’s observation that we use our dominant hands for approach-oriented actions, while nondominant hands are used for avoidance movements.
“You would wield the sword in your dominant hand to make approach-related actions like stabbing your enemy, and use the shield in your nondominant hand to fend off attack,” he says. “Your dominant hand gets the thing you want and your nondominant hand pushes away the thing you don’t.”
The researchers theorized that approach and avoidance emotions are built on neural systems for approach and avoidance actions.
“If this sword and shield hypothesis is correct,” he says, “then three things should follow: Approach motivation should be mediated by the left hemisphere in strong right-handers, as it has been in tons of previous studies. But it should completely reverse in strong left-handers. For everyone in the middle of the handedness spectrum, approach emotions should depend on both hemispheres.”
Life-affirming emotions
Researchers tested this idea by stimulating the two hemispheres of the brains of 25 healthy participants with a pain-free electrical current. The goal was to see if they could cause the participants to experience approach-related emotions—including enthusiasm, interest, strength, excitement, determination, and alertness—depending on which hemisphere of the brain was stimulated and whether they were righties or lefties or somewhere in between.
The study participants got zapped for 20 minutes a day for five days, and reported before and after the five days how strongly they were feeling emotions like pride and happiness.
The experiment worked—and corroborated the researchers’ first test of the sword and shield hypothesis using brain imaging. Strong righties who researchers zapped in the left hemisphere experienced a boost in positive emotions. So did strong lefties zapped in the right hemisphere. But when lefties are zapped in the left hemisphere—or righties in the right—”you see either no change or a detriment in the experience of these emotions,” Casasanto says.
Mental health narratives matter for marginalized young people
The work has implications for a current treatment for recalcitrant anxiety and depression called neural therapy. Similar to the technique used in the study and approved by the Food and Drug Administration, it involves a mild electrical stimulation or a magnetic stimulation to the left side of the brain, to encourage approach-related emotions.
But as reported in Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, the treatment could be damaging for left-handed patients. Stimulation on the left would decrease life-affirming approach emotions. “If you give left-handers the standard treatment, you’re probably going to make them worse,” Casasanto says.
“And because many people are neither strongly right- nor left-handed, the stimulation won’t make any difference for them, because their approach emotions are distributed across both hemispheres,” he says.
“This suggests strong righties should get the normal treatment, but they make up only 50 percent of the population. Strong lefties should get the opposite treatment, and people in the middle shouldn’t get the treatment at all.”
Prehistoric teeth offer early evidence for right-handedness
Casasanto cautions the research studied only healthy participants and more work is needed to extend these findings to a clinical setting.
A James S. McDonnell Foundation Scholar Award and the National Science Foundation funded the work.
Source: Cornell University