When the circumstances are right, people can think like computers, researchers report.
Computers, like those that power self-driving cars, can mistake random scribbles for trains, fences, and even school busses. People aren’t supposed to be able to see how those images trip up computers but in a new study, researchers show most people actually can.
The findings suggest modern computers may not be as different from humans as we think, and demonstrate how advances in artificial intelligence continue to narrow the gap between the visual abilities of people and machines.
“Most of the time, research in our field is about getting computers to think like people,” says senior author Chaz Firestone, an assistant professor in the psychological and brain sciences department at Johns Hopkins University. “Our project does the opposite—we’re asking whether people can think like computers.”
Fooling A.I.
What’s easy for humans is often hard for computers. Artificial intelligence systems have long been better than people at doing math or remembering large quantities of information; but for decades humans have had the edge at recognizing everyday objects such as dogs, cats, tables, or chairs.
In other cases, machines see armadillos and bagels in what looks like meaningless TV static.
But recently, “neural networks” that mimic the brain have approached the human ability to identify objects, leading to technological advances supporting self-driving cars, facial recognition programs, and helping physicians to spot abnormalities in radiological scans.
But even with these technological advances, there’s a critical blind spot: It’s possible to purposely make images that neural networks cannot correctly see. And these images, called “adversarial” or “fooling” images, are a big problem: Not only could hackers exploit them and cause security risks, but they suggest that humans and machines are actually seeing images very differently.
In some cases, all it takes for a computer to call an apple a car, is reconfiguring a pixel or two. In other cases, machines see armadillos and bagels in what looks like meaningless television static.
“These machines seem to be misidentifying objects in ways humans never would,” Firestone says. “But surprisingly, nobody has really tested this. How do we know people can’t see what the computers did?”
‘Think like a machine’
To test this, Firestone and lead author Zhenglong Zhou, a senior majoring in cognitive science, essentially asked people to “think like a machine”. Machines have only a relatively small vocabulary for naming images. So, Firestone and Zhou showed people dozens of fooling images that had already tricked computers, and gave people the same kinds of labeling options that the machine had.
In particular, they asked people which of two options the computer decided the object was—one being the computer’s real conclusion and the other a random answer. (Was the blob pictured a bagel or a pinwheel?) It turns out, people strongly agreed with the conclusions of the computers.
People chose the same answer as computers 75 percent of the time. Perhaps even more remarkably, 98 percent of people tended to answer like the computers did.
Next, researchers upped the ante by giving people a choice between the computer’s favorite answer and its next-best guess. (Was the blob pictured a bagel or a pretzel?) People again validated the computer’s choices, with 91 percent of test participants agreeing with the machine’s first choice.
Even when the researchers had people guess between 48 choices for what the object was, and even when the pictures resembled television static, an overwhelming proportion of the subjects chose what the machine chose well above the rates for random chance. Researchers tested a total of 1,800 subjects throughout the various experiments.
“We found if you put a person in the same circumstance as a computer, suddenly the humans tend to agree with the machines,” Firestone says.
“This is still a problem for artificial intelligence, but it’s not like the computer is saying something completely unlike what a human would say.”
The research appears in the journal Nature Communications.
Source: Johns Hopkins University