How individual people behave during an outbreak of the flu or some other illness can affect the progress of the disease, but perhaps not in the way you’d suspect.
“When we studied individual behavior, empathy trumped risk aversion in disease eradication, which was counterintuitive for us,” says Ceyhun Eksin, a postdoctoral fellow in the Georgia Tech School of Biological Sciences.
Eksin and colleagues used a networked variation of game theory to pit the self-interests of susceptible healthy people against those of infected people. They found that healthy people attempting to protect themselves couldn’t, by themselves, stop the disease from spreading. Only if sick people took precautions to avoid infecting others could the illness be eradicated.
“A little bit of empathy can be crucial at this point.”
Among the key factors was empathy of infected people.
“We wanted to understand disease dynamics from an individual’s perspective,” says Eksin. “In particular, we wanted to know what role individual behavior plays in disease spread and how behavior might affect forecasting and consequences in the long run when there is an outbreak.”
Public health initiatives against seasonal diseases like influenza tend to initially focus on immunization programs, which move individuals out of the “susceptible” category. Once an outbreak begins, health campaigns focus on encouraging susceptible persons to take precautions such as hand-washing and avoiding infected people.
The success of those measures may depend on individual perceptions of how great the risk of infection might be, Eksin says. The more awareness individuals have of infected persons around them, the more likely they are to protect themselves.
Sick people at work
Perception can also affect the behavior of infected individuals, who may be more likely to stay home from work or cover their cough, for instance, if they believe their presence could infect a significant number of people.
“If an infected person really wants to attend a meeting at work, it’s one thing if only one other person could be at risk,” he says. “It may be a different thing if they could affect a whole office of susceptible people.”
The empathy becomes especially important toward the end of an outbreak, when the number of infected persons may be low, but still enough to sustain the illness. When the number of sick people is low, risk perception falls, leading susceptible people to reduce their precautions—and sick people to feel less concern about infecting others.
If those sick persons then decide to head off to work despite their illness, they may infect unsuspecting susceptible people, causing the outbreak to continue.
“The behavior of the infected individuals can be more important than the behavior of the susceptible individuals in eradicating the disease,” says Eksin. “A little bit of empathy can be crucial at this point.”
While the research examined the results of individual actions during an outbreak, those individual decisions were actually related because individuals are part of networks of contacts.
“In a connected network, everyone’s activities can affect you in one way or another,” Eksin says. “If your sick neighbors decide to isolate themselves, then you don’t need to take any action to protect yourself against them.
“But you don’t necessarily know what your neighbors are doing, and their actions may well be affected by what others are doing. The effect can cascade.”
The Army Research Office supported the work. Results appear in the journal Scientific Reports.
Source: Georgia Tech